Brain & Nervous System Neurological Symptoms & Diagnosis Synesthesia and the Involuntary Blending of Senses By Peter Pressman, MD Updated on September 20, 2021 Medically reviewed by Diana Apetauerova, MD Print What is the sound of yellow? What color is the number 3? For most people, these questions may seem whimsical, nonsensical, or maybe poetic. Other people can answer from their own experience. People with synesthesia have an automatic and involuntary blending of senses. The term synesthesia comes from the Greek for syn (together) and anesthesia (sensation). Acoustic information may become olfactory, for example, so that music has a certain smell. While almost any type of sensory blending is possible, some forms are more commonly reported than others. While no form of synesthesia is exactly common, some of the best-described forms are described below. Synesthesia. Danm / Getty Images Types Grapheme to Color: Certain letters or numbers (graphemes) appear tinged with color in this most common form of synesthesia. Certain similarities are common between individuals: for example, the letter A is usually red. A special variation is called spatial sequence synesthesia, in which people see the grapheme as positioned differently in space. For example, the letter 3 may be further away than the number 4.Sound to Color: People with sound to color synesthesia may see something like colorful fireworks triggered by different sounds in their environment, like voices, music, or traffic. Some people only have the experience for a particular type of sound, while others experience the fireworks with many different noises.Number Forms: When some people picture numbers, they appear in different shapes that connect the numbers together.Ordinal Linguistic Personification: Anything that appears in a sequence, such as days, months, letters or numbers, may become associated with different personalities. Sometimes objects may be imbued with personality as well. Because this isn't a direct sensory association, some people have questioned its inclusion with other synesthesia types. It is automatic and unintentional, however, which leads many to request its inclusion.Lexical to Gustatory Synesthesia: In this rare form of synesthesia, words have a distinctive taste. The word "computer," for example, could taste like a chocolate bar. Sometimes the food taste depends on the letters of the word. For example, the letter c could taste like chocolate.Auditory-Tactile Synesthesia: This is another rare form of synesthesia, in which certain sounds can induce sensations in parts of the body.Mirror Touch Synesthesia: This is yet another rare form in which you literally feel what another person feels. If you see someone get physically hurt, you will feel actual pain in the same location. Who Experiences Synesthesia While many people may experience synesthesia through the use of drugs such as LSD, it's not certain how many people experience synesthesia naturally. Estimates range widely from about one in 20 to one in 2000 people. Early studies suggest that it is more common in women than men, though more recent samples suggest the prevalence is almost equal between genders. Synesthesia can run in families, but may also result from stroke, seizure, or as a result of sensory loss due to blindness or deafness. How Synesthesia Is Studied Part of the problem of researching synesthesia is a reliance on people's description of their experience. Psychological tests, though, can be used to verify what people say about their synesthesia. For example, the letter A might be sprinkled in with hundreds of other letters on a piece of paper. Someone with grapheme to color synesthesia will find those letters much faster than someone without, because to the synesthete all of those letters appear red in color. A variation of this has been tested with the letter S and the number 2. Causes Everything we experience exists as patterns of flowing electrical signals in the brain. Usually, different regions of the brain represent different types of information. The occipital lobes contain information about vision, for example, and part of the temporal lobes contain information about sound. Synesthesia may be caused by unusual cases of cross-talk between normally separated brain regions. This might explain why grapheme to color synesthesia is relatively common among synesthetes. Graphemes are thought to be represented at the junction between the parietal and temporal lobes. Information about color is located relatively nearby. This means that it might be easier for certain information to get mixed up. When we are young, we have more brain connections than we do when we get older. There is a pruning process of neural connections that probably ultimately helps us to make sense of the world. Synesthesia might be due to insufficient pruning. Another theory is that information overlap is usually held in check by tonic inhibitory mechanisms in the brain. When this inhibition is removed, synesthesia can result. This might explain the synesthetic effects of certain drugs, as well as some seizures or strokes. Is Synthesthesia a Neurological Disorder? While synesthesia comes from the neurological processes of the brain, it would be unfair to call it a disorder. Synthesthesia is usually not bothersome. It is simply a different way of perceiving the world. Many people with synesthesia do not recognize their experiences as being unusual until it becomes obvious that other people don't have the same experiences, which often occurs in childhood. Many find that people with synesthesia may be more creative. In fact, all of us have brain processes that are similar to synesthesia. For example, one study found that if shown a sharp, irregular shape, or a rounded curvaceous blob, we are more likely to say the first is called "kiki" and the second "boubua," regardless of any prior training. Our brain automatically makes associations between things that would seem to have no reason for that connection. In short, synesthesia is not only a unique way of perceiving the way around us; it also lets us better perceive what occurs within us, in the workings of our brain. Sources Verywell Health uses only high-quality sources, including peer-reviewed studies, to support the facts within our articles. Read our editorial process to learn more about how we fact-check and keep our content accurate, reliable, and trustworthy. Banissy MJ, Ward J. Mirror-Touch Synesthesia Is Linked With Empathy. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10(7):815-6. Baron-cohen S, Harrison J, Goldstein LH, Wyke M. Coloured Speech Perception: Is Synaesthesia What Happens When Modularity Breaks Down?. Perception 22. 1993;22(4):419-26. C van Campen (2007). The Hidden Sense: Synesthesia in Art and Science. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. Hubbard EM, Arman AC, Ramachandran VS, Boynton GM. Individual Differences Among Grapheme-Color Synesthetes: Brain-Behavior Correlations. Neuron. 2005;45(6):975-85. J Simner, C Mulvenna, N Sagiv, E Tsakanitos, SA Witherby, C Fraser, K Scott, J Ward. Synaethesia: The Prevalence of Atypical Cross-Modeal Experiences. (2006) Perception 35: 1024-1033. MW Calkins (1893). A Statistical Study of Pseudo-Chromesthesia and of Mental-Forms. The American Journal of Psychology (University of Illinois Press) 5 (4): 439-64. doi:10.2307/1411912 By Peter Pressman, MD Peter Pressman, MD, is a board-certified neurologist developing new ways to diagnose and care for people with neurocognitive disorders. See Our Editorial Process Meet Our Medical Expert Board Share Feedback Was this page helpful? Thanks for your feedback! What is your feedback? Other Helpful Report an Error Submit